Kanzi: The Ape at the Brink of the Human Mind (35 page)

BOOK: Kanzi: The Ape at the Brink of the Human Mind
4.14Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads

Traditionally, language interventionists have pursued two different routes to structured teaching: One is remedial, the other developmental. The remedial model supposes that children “being taught language relatively late in their lives, because they have failed to acquire it adequately in their earlier experience, no longer possess the same collection of abilities and deficits that normal children have when they begin to acquire language.”
3
This very pragmatic approach attempts to teach useful communication skills as quickly as possible, without consideration of the prelinguistic or cognitive skills the child might already possess.

The developmental model, on the other hand, is driven very much by theory, specifically, theory based on the observations of nonimpaired children. According to this approach, the impaired child, in learning communication skills, must pass through the same steps that a normal child spontaneously does. But as these steps are at best incompletely understood due to the largely hidden nature of normal language acquisition, only limited guidance can be developed for teaching strategies.

Neither the remedial model nor the developmental model is of much help to those children who completely fail to speak. Such children have often been given up as hopeless cases, but it is here that the experience of teaching language skills to chimpanzees has been especially fruitful. Earlier chapters of this book have shown how our language-teaching strategies with apes evolved through time, as our experience increased and we understood more about the components of language and how they were learned. We progressed from the insights of the LANA project, through Sherman and Austin, and on to Kanzi and the other bonobos. Advances in the work with humans tracked those with chimps every step of the way, often surprisingly so.

Duane initiated the first of his studies with humans in 1975, in conjunction with the Georgia Retardation Center. Nine institutionalized individuals took part in the study. They were aged between eleven years, eleven months and eighteen years, three months, and were all profoundly retarded. Only one of them spoke in any way, this individual producing some ten word-approximations that were unintelligible to the naive listener. Traditional speech-language treatment had failed with these individuals, and they had been judged to have insufficient prelinguistic skills to warrant participation in a language intervention program. “We stacked the cards against us by working with severely handicapped individuals,” Duane now recalls. “The firm expectation by staff at the Georgia Retardation Center was that they would learn nothing at all in our program either. So, if we did make progress, no matter how small, that would have to be recognized as important.”
4

Five of the original nine people in the study continued in it for more than three years, and all of them did learn how to use symbols in a simple, communicative way. The size of the acquired vocabulary varied considerably, with a range of twenty to seventy-five symbols. With LANA-based teaching, the program achieved LANA-like abilities in the subjects, namely, the apparently competent symbol production but rudimentary comprehension. Like Lana, Washoe, Sarah, and Nim, these children learned to make their wishes known and to answer simple questions by stringing symbols together. Yet they also suffered similar deficiencies. Although they could answer questions with words, they often could not respond to commands with appropriate behavior. For example, Sam might say
Yes want cookie
if asked whether he was hungry, but if asked to go to the table and get a cookie, he often failed to respond. Nevertheless, the program had achieved its aim: to test the feasibility of using the computer-based keyboard system to teach symbol association and use to nonspeaking, mentally retarded children.

Duane believes that some of the physical characteristics of the communicative system—such as the striking shapes of lexigrams, their colors, and the fact that they lit up when selected—were important in its unexpected success. This
helped gain the children’s attention, which is vital but difficult when teaching mentally retarded children. Duane’s insight of teaching with a computer-based keyboard lexigram system had been vindicated, and it soon became possible to pursue the concept further by applying what I had been learning with Sherman and Austin.

An important conclusion from my work with Sherman and Austin was that chimpanzees are capable of using symbols referentially, as humans do, but that the skill emerges only through the systematic teaching of the various components of language. These components include requesting, naming, and comprehension. The individuals in the original collaborative study with the Georgia Retardation Center had acquired languagelike skills, but they lacked a strong referential quality. The LANA project had provided the model for teaching and, inevitably, LANA-like performance was the result. The obvious step to take was to implement a teaching paradigm based on what I had learned with Sherman and Austin, with the hope of developing symbolic communication among the human students. Mary Ann and Rose initiated such a program at the Language Research Center with James Pate, beginning in 1981. The program was run in collaboration with the Developmental Learning Center of Georgia Regional Hospital.

The computer-based communication system would remain essentially the same as in the earlier study, but in this case there would be far more emphasis on comprehension. As Mary Ann and her colleagues wrote in a description of the project, “Symbolic communication is a complex phenomenon requiring an individual to comprehend the meanings of symbols, to produce symbols in appropriate situations, and to use arbitrary symbols for interindividual communication.”
5
Important in the new study was a set of operationally defined components of symbolic communication. They are:

  1. An arbitrary symbol that stands for, and takes the place of, a real object, event, person, action, or relationship.
  2. Stored knowledge regarding the actions, objects, and relationships relating to that symbol.
  3. The intentional use of symbols to convey this stored knowledge about an object, event, person, action, or relationship to another individual who has similar real-world experiences and has related them to the same symbol system.
  4. The appropriate decoding of, and response to, symbols by the recipients.

The most fundamental lesson in acquiring language is learning that symbols have a function in communication. As Mary Ann and her colleagues point out, for severely retarded individuals, “this first step toward learning to communicate symbolically is perhaps the most difficult to achieve, regardless of the modality in which the instruction occurs.”
6
A teaching paradigm based on my experience with Sherman and Austin was designed to surmount this difficulty, moving step by step. The first step was to teach the request function.

In unimpaired children, requesting visible objects is one of the earliest communicative actions to occur in development, with other skills emerging rapidly thereafter. Language comprehension begins as early as three to six months after birth, and by one to one and a half years, children spontaneously begin to make their needs known. In children who suffer brain impairment, both comprehension and production may fail to occur. Previous attempts to train such children to communicate have typically focused on symbol production, as there seemed to be no efficient means of measuring progress in language understanding. Additionally, most teachers have assumed that if such a child learns to use words, he or she will automatically understand them. However, a major problem with approaches that fostered symbol production was that while children learned words, they often failed to use them readily outside the training situation. Thus, though the children acquired words such as “potty” or “thank you,” they nonetheless failed to tell their teachers in all circumstances when they needed to go to the
potty or even to ask where it was located when placed in a new setting.

Mary Ann wondered if such generalization failures resulted from a lack of comprehension skills. Perhaps these children had learned what to say to gain the teacher’s approval, much as Washoe and Lana learned what to say to win the approval of their caretakers. Could it be that these children, like apes, did not really understand that words did more than get them things? Did they realize that words were being used in all manner of ways set by others, and that to “catch on” to this they had to listen and decipher the meaning or intent behind each utterance that others made? The meaning or intent of others was often very different from one’s own and had to be “figured out” anew for each occasion.

Mary Ann decided to try to determine whether it was possible to give such children an introduction to language that provided them with a thorough grounding in the myriad of different ways that symbols function—as a means of asking for things, naming things, making statements about intended actions, and understanding and cooperating with others.

Four individuals took part in the study: Bev and Connie, whom I mentioned earlier, and Ruth (aged eighteen years, eleven months) and Max (aged fourteen years, one month). Like Bev and Connie, Ruth and Max had failed to acquire speech or augmentative communication, despite extensive teaching efforts. Because of their ages (relatively old) and absence of progress in traditional language-teaching regimes, the four individuals were reckoned by their caregivers to have little chance with the Sherman and Austin regime, either. Mary Ann and Rose, however, were confident of making progress.

In order to establish as strong a motivation as possible in the teaching regime, Mary Ann and her colleagues used favorite food items as objects to be requested. This would provide a strong connection between the use of a symbol and the consequences of that use. The subjects were to be introduced to one food item and its lexigram at the beginning of the program; when the association between lexigram and food item had been learned, a second would be introduced; and so on. Mary Ann
and her colleagues established a series of stages of learning each lexigram, until the subject could accurately pick it out among a field of other lexigrams. Bev learned quickly, requiring only 125 trials to reach the required accuracy for the first lexigram. Connie and Ruth were much slower, requiring 469 and 903 trials, while Max never really succeeded, even after more than 2500 trials. He was excluded from the study at that point.

The difficulty that the subjects faced with the first lexigram paled against the experience with the addition of a second. The number of trials Connie and Ruth required for the establishment of accurate requesting with this second lexigram were 2217 and 1319, respectively. Even Bev found the task more difficult, requiring 298. Thereafter, however, the subjects found the learning task much easier. For instance, Connie required 102 and 455 trials for learning the third and fourth lexigrams, respectively; for Ruth, the figures were 262 and 337; and for Bev, 93 and 42. “They learn how to learn,” observed Mary Ann, “and the quality of what they learn improves.”
7
One mark of this improvement was the subjects’ eventual production of multilexigram utterances. As Elizabeth Bates has pointed out, in mentally normal individuals, the transition from one-word to multiword utterances is the third milestone in the acquisition of language. (The first is the emergence of communicative intentionality and the second is the appearance of reference.) Bev, Connie, and Ruth can therefore be considered to have reached that milestone.

Mary Ann and her colleagues fostered naming and comprehension skills as the subjects learned each new lexigram by following the same procedures employed with Sherman and Austin five years earlier. Bev, Connie, and Ruth were like Sherman and Austin in that naming and comprehension skills needed to be fostered by specific training. At the time, we believed that this said something fundamental about the nature of language and how it must be put in place in ape and impaired-human brains. But as we were later to discover through our experience with Kanzi, it had at least as much to do with the teaching paradigm we used.

Nevertheless, the study was important at the time, and
demonstrated that “initiating instruction at the level of communicative request is a viable beginning for the establishment of symbolic communication in persons with mental retardation who have severe oral language impairments,” concluded Mary Ann, Rose, and James. “The four operational components of symbolic communication were observed concurrently after these individuals progressed into Phase 2 of the study”
8

Not only did labeling and comprehension skills emerge as the subjects’ capacity for symbolic communication emerged, but so too did other language-related behaviors, such as spontaneous initiation of communication and an improvement in comprehension of spoken words. In Bev’s case, improvement in her own speech also emerged. When Bev began the program she had been unable to comprehend the English words for any of the lexigrams she eventually was able to use. But as she learned to use the lexigrams, she also comprehended the spoken word for them, doubtless because Mary Ann and her colleagues also spoke the word relating to a particular lexigram when it was indicated. This comprehension seemed to drive Bev’s attempts to vocalize the lexigram words. Two words were clear, namely, “apple” and “Bev”; for the rest, she produced bisyllabic approximations where previously her utterances had typically been monosyllabic. Although Connie eventually comprehended five spoken versions of the lexigram set, she produced only a
ma
or
ba
vocalization when asked “What’s this?” Ruth was hindered by severe hearing impairment, and neither comprehended English words nor produced word approximations.

BOOK: Kanzi: The Ape at the Brink of the Human Mind
4.14Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads

Other books

For Real (Rules of Love, Book One) by Cameron, Chelsea M.
Skating Around The Law by Joelle Charbonneau
Peter and Alice by John Logan
Polly's Pride by Freda Lightfoot
The Lonely Ones by Kelsey Sutton
Provoked by Zanetti, Rebecca